Learning behaviors, executive functions, and social skills: A meta-analysis on the factors influencing learning development in the transition from kindergarten to elementary school
Henriette Offer-Boljahn 1 * , Dennis Christian Hövel 2, Thomas Hennemann 1
More Detail
1 University of Cologne, Faculty of Human Sciences, Department of Health Education and Rehabilitation, Germany
2 University of Applied Sciences in Special Needs Education (HfH), Institut für Verhalten, sozio-emotionale und psychomotorische, Entwicklungsförderung, Switzerland
* Corresponding Author

Abstract

The linguistic, mathematical, social-emotional, and cognitive precursor competencies are important predictors of learning success already at kindergarten age. A systematic analysis of the state of research on the actual interrelationships of the focused precursor competencies brings together results of a meta-analysis. The literature search yielded 653 hits, which were reduced to 94 hits after applying exclusion criteria. It was possible to perform 271 correlation tests in 39 pathways. Thus, the sample (n = 141,508  [M = 1505, SD = 3930 ]) with an average age of 5.2 years (SD = 0.10  years) is balanced in gender ratio of 48.5 % male and 48.9 % female (2.6 % not specified). The largest correlation between literacy and mathematical precursor skills is r = .65 . With a correlation between r = .17  and r = .60 , executive functions significantly influence the development of literacy and mathematical skills and learning behaviors. Learning behaviors are most strongly related to social skills, with r = .57 . Parental influence turns out to be smaller, with correlations ranging from r = .19  to .55 . The effect of individual pathways is also small, and peer group was not included in the search term combination. Successful educational processes in day care centers can be improved with a focus on the promotion of executive functions and learning behaviors.

Keywords

References

  • *Ahmed, S. F., Tang, S., Waters, N. E., & Davis-Kean, P. (2018). Executive function and academic achievement: Longitudinal relations from early childhood to adolescence. Journal of Educational Psychology, 111(3), 446–458. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000296
  • *Alloway, T. P., & Alloway, R. G. (2010). Investigating the predictive roles of working memory and IQ in academic attainment. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 106(1), 20–29. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2009.11.003
  • *Alloway, T. P., Gathercole, S. E., Adams, A.-M., Willies, C., Eaglen, R., & Lamont, E. (2005). Working memory and phonological awareness as predictors of progress towards early learning goals at school entry. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23(3), 417–426. https://doi.org/10.1348/026151005X26804
  • *Anders, Y., Rossbach, H.-G., Weintert, S., Ebert, S., Kuger, S., Lehrl, S., & Maurice, J. (2012). Home and preschool learning environments and their relations to the development of early numeracy skills. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 27(2), 231–244. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2011.08.003
  • *Aragón, E., Navarro, J., Aguilar, M., Cerda, G., & García-Sedeño, M. (2016). Predictive model for early math skills based on structural equations. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 57(6), 489–494. https://doi.org/10.1111/sjop.12317
  • *Arnold, D. H., Kupersmidt, J. B., Voegler-Lee, M. E., & Marshall, N. A. (2012). The association between preschool children’s social functioning and their emergent academic skills. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 27(3), 376–386. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2011.12.009
  • *Austin, A. M., & Blevins-Knabe, B. (2012). Early mathematics and phonological awareness in two child care settings. Early Child Development and Care, 183(9), 1–18. https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2012.716832
  • Baddeley, A. (1986). Working memory. Clarendon Press.
  • Bader, S., Bloem, S., Riedel, B., Seybel, C., & Turani, D. (Eds.). (2019). Kita-Praxis im internationalen Vergleich. Ergebnisse der OECD-Fachkräftebefragung 2018 [Daycare Practices in International Comparison. Results of the OECD Teacher Survey 2018]. https://www.dji.de/fileadmin/user_upload/bibs2019/28314_Kita-Praxis_im_internationalen_Vergleich.pdf
  • Blair, C., & Raver, C. C. (2014). Closing the achievement gap through modification of neurocognitive and neuroendocrine function: Results from a cluster randomized controlled trial of an innovative approach to the education of children in kindergarten. PLoS ONE, 9(11), 1–13. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0112393
  • Blewitt, C., Fuller-Tyszkiewicz, M., Nolan, A., Bergmeier, H., Vicary, D., Huang, T., McCabe, P., McKay, T., & Skouteris, H. (2018). Social and emotional learning associated with universal curriculum-based interventions in early childhood education and care centers. A systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Network Open, 1, 1-19. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2018.5727
  • Bredel, U. (2016). Schriftspracherwerb. In U. Domahs, & B. Rimus (Eds.), Handbuch Laut, Gebärde, Buchstabe [Handbook Sound, Gesture, Letter] (pp. 436-454). de Gruyter.
  • *Bull, R., Espy, K. A., & Wiebe, S. A. (2008). Short-term memory, working memory, and executive functioning in preschoolers: Longitudinal predictors of mathematical achievement at age 7 years. Developmental Neuropsychology, 33(3), 205–228. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565640801982312
  • *Bull, R., Espy, K. A., Wiebe, S. A., Sheffield, T. D., & Nelson, J. M. (2011). Using confirmatory factor analysis to understand executive control in preschool children: Sources of variation in emergent mathematic achievement. Developmental Science, 14(4), 679–692. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.01012.x
  • *Bulotsky-Shearer, R. J., Fernandez, V., Dominguez, X., & Rouse, H. L. (2011). Behavior problems in learning activities and social interactions in head start classrooms and early reading, mathematics, and approaches to learning. School Psychology Review, 40(1), 39–56. https://doi.org/10.1080/02796015.2011.12087727
  • *Byrnes, J. P., Wang, A., & Miller-Cotto, D. (2019). Children as mediators of their own cognitive development in kindergarten. Cognitive Development, 50, 80–97. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2019.03.003
  • *Chrysochoou, E., & Bablekou, Z. (2011). Phonological loop and central executive contributions to oral comprehension skills of 5.5 to 9.5 years old children. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 25(4), 576–583. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1723
  • *Cirino, P. T. (2011). The interrelationships of mathematical precursors in kindergarten. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108(4), 713–733. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2010.11.004
  • *Clark, C. A., Pritchard, V. E., & Woodward, L. J. (2010). Preschool executive functioning abilities predict early mathematics achievement. Developmental Psychology, 46(5), 1176–1191. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019672
  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Döring, N., & Bortz, J. (2016). Forschungsmethoden und Evaluation in den Sozial- und Humanwissenschaften [Research Methods and Evaluation in Social and Human Sciences] (5th ed.). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-41089-5
  • *Davidse, N. J., De Jong, M. T., & Bus, A. G. (2014). Explaining common variance shared by early numeracy and literacy. Reading and Writing, 27, 631–648. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-013-9465-0
  • Duncan, G. J., Dowsett, C. J., Claessens, A., Magnuson, K., Huston, A. C., Klebanov, P., Pagani, L. S., Feinstein, L., Engel, M., Brooks-Gunn, J., Sexton, H., Duckworth, K., & Japel, C. (2007). School readiness and later achievement. Developmental Psychology, 43(6), 1428–1446. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.43.6.1428
  • *Elliott, L. (2019). Profiles of academic, cognitive, and behavioral development from kindergarten to third grade. Learning and Individual Differences, 70, 109–120. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2019.01.010
  • Faust, G., Kratzmann, J., & Wehner, F. (2012). School Entry – a Risk for Schoolbeginners? Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie, 26(3), 197–212. https://doi.org/10.1024/1010-0652/a000069
  • *Ferretti, L., & Bub, K. L. (2017). Family routines and school readiness during the transition to kindergarten. Early Education and Development, 28(1), 59–77. https://doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2016.1195671
  • *Forget-Dubois, N., Lemelin, J.-P., Boivin, M., Dionne, G., Séguin, J. R., Vitaro, F., & Tremblay, R. E. (2007). Predicting early school achievement with the EDI: A longitudinal population-based study. Early Education and Development, 18(3), 405–426. https://doi.org/10.1080/10409280701610796
  • *Foster, M. E., Anthony, J. L., Clements, D. H., & Sarama, J. H. (2015). Processes in the development of mathematics in kindergarten children from Title 1 schools. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 140, 56–73. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.07.004
  • *Friedman, S. L., Scholnick, E. K., Bender, R. H., Vandergrift, N., Spieker, S., Hirsh Pasek, K., Keating, D. P., & Park, Y. (2014). Planning in middle childhood: Early predictors and later outcomes. Child Development, 85(4), 1446–1460. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12221
  • *Fuchs, L. S., Geary, D. C., Compton, D. L., Fuchs, D., Hamlett, C. L., & Bryant, J. D. (2010). The contributions of numerosity and domain-general abilities to school readiness. Child Development, 81(5), 1520–1533. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01489.x
  • *Fuhs, M. W. (2011). Verbal ability and executive functioning development in preschoolers at Head Start. Developmental Psychology, 47(2), 404–416. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021065
  • *Fuhs, M. W., Farran, D. C., & Nesbitt, K. T. (2015). Prekindergarten children’s executive functioning skills and achievement gains: The utility of direct assessments and teacher ratings. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107(1), 207–221. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037366
  • *Fuhs, M. W., Nesbitt, K. T., Farran, D. C., & Dong, N. (2014). Longitudinal associations between executive functioning and academic skills across content areas. Developmental Psychology, 50(1), 1698–1709. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036633
  • Gasteiger-Klicpera, B., Klicpera, C., & Schabmann, A. (2006). Relationship between reading, spelling and behavior disorders: A longitudinal study from kindergarten to the fourth grade of elementary school. Kindheit und Entwicklung, 15(1), 55–67. https://doi.org/10.1026/0942-5403.15.1.55
  • *Geary, D. C., & vanMarle, K. (2016). Young children’s core symbolic and nonsymbolic quantitative knowledge in the prediction of later mathematics achievement. Developmental Psychology, 52(12), 2130–2144. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000214
  • *Georgiou, G. K., Tziraki, N., Manolitsis, G., & Fella, A. (2013). Is rapid automatized naming related to reading and mathematics for the same reason(s)? A follow-up study from kindergarten to Grade 1. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 115(3), 481–496. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2013.01.004
  • *Georgiou, G. K., Wei, W., Inoue, T., Das, J. P., & Deng, C. (2019). Cultural influences on the relation between executive functions and academic achievement. Reading and Writing, 33, 991–1013. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-019-09961-8
  • Gold, A. (2018). Lernschwierigkeiten. Ursachen, Diagnostik, Intervention [Learning Disorders. Causes, Diagnostics, Interventions] (2nd ed.). Stuttgart: Kohlhammer.
  • *Graziano, P. A., Reavis, R. D., Keane, S. P., & Calkins, S. D. (2007). The role of emotion regulation and children's early academic success. Journal of School Psychology, 45(1), 3–19. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2006.09.002
  • Hagarty, I., & Morgan, G. (2020). Social-emotional learning for children with learning disabilities: A systematic review. Educational Psychology in Practice, 36(2), 208–222. https://doi.org/10.1080/02667363.2020.1742096
  • Hasselhorn, M., & Grube, D. (2008). Individual Preconditions and Developmental Constraints of Children’s Learning during the Preschool and Early School Years. Empirische Pädagogik, 22, 113–126.
  • *Hassinger-Das, B., Jordan, N. C., Glutting, J., Irwin, C., & Dyson, N. (2014). Domain general mediators of the relation between kindergarten number sense and first-grade mathematics achievement. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 118, 78–92. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2013.09.008
  • *Hirsch, S., Lambert, K., Coppens, K., & Moeller, K. (2018). Basic numerical competences in large-scale assessment data: Structure and long-term relevance. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 167, 32–48. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.09.015
  • Hohm, E., Laucht, M., Zohsel, K., Schmidt, M., Brandeis, G., & Banaschweski, T. (2017). Resilience and resources during development: From early childhood to adulthood. Kindheit und Entwicklung, 26(4), 230–239. https://doi.org/10.1026/0942-5403/a000236
  • *Hornburg, C. B., Schmitt, S. A., & Purpura, D. J. (2018). Relations between preschoolers’ mathematical language understanding and specific numeracy skills. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 176, 84–100. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2018.07.005
  • *Hughes, J., & Kwok, O.-M. (2007). Influence of student-teacher and parent-teacher relationships on lower achieving readers’ engagement and achievement in the primary grades. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99(1), 39–51. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.99.1.39
  • *Hughes, J., Luo, W., Kwok, O.-M., & Loyd, L. K. (2008). Teacher-student support, effortful engagement, and achievement: A 3-year longitudinal study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100(1), 1–14. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.100.1.1
  • *Isbell, E., Calkins, S. D., Cole, V. T., Swingler, M. M., & Leerkes, E. M. (2019). Longitudinal associations between conflict monitoring and emergent academic skills: An event‐related potentials study. Developmental Psychobiology, 61(4), 495–512. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.21809
  • *Kleemans, T., Peeters, M., Segers, E., & Verhoeven, L. (2012). Child and home predictors of early numeracy skills in kindergarten. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 27(3), 471–477. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2011.12.004
  • *Kleemans, T., Segers, E., & Verhoeven, L. (2011). Cognitive and linguistic precursors to numeracy in kindergarten: Evidence from first and second language learners. Learning and Individual Differences, 21(5), 555–561. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2011.07.008
  • *Koponen, T., Aunola, K., Ahonen, T., & Nurmi, J.-E. (2007). Cognitive predictors of single-digit and procedural calculation skills and their covariation with reading skill. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 97(3), 220–241. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2007.03.001
  • *Koponen, T., Salmi, P., Torppa, M., Eklund, K., Aro, T., Aro, M., Poikkeus, A.-M., Lerkkanen, M.-K., & Nurmi, J.-E. (2016). Counting and rapid naming predict the fluency of arithmetic and reading skills. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 44-45, 83–94. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2016.02.004
  • *Krajewski, K., Schneider, W., & Nieding, G. (2008). Early Training of Quantity – Number Competencies in Preschool. Psychologie in Erziehung und Unterricht, 55, 100–113.
  • *Kyttälä, M., Aunio, P., Lepola, J., & Hautamäki, J. (2013). The role of the working memory and language skills in the prediction of word problem solving in 4- to 7-year-old children. Educational Psychology, 34(6), 674–696. https://doi.org/10.1080/01443410.2013.814192
  • *LeFevre, J.-A., Fast, L., Skwarchuk, S.-L., Smith-Chant, B., Bisanz, J., Kamawar, D., & Penner-Wilger, M. (2010). Pathways to mathematics: Longitudinal predictors of performance. Child Development, 81(6), 1753–1767. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01508.x
  • *Lepola, J., Niemi, P., Kuikka, M., & Hannula, M. M. (2005). Cognitive-linguistic skills and motivation as longitudinal predictors of reading and arithmetic achievement: A follow-up study from kindergarten to grade 2. International Journal of Educational Research, 43(4-5), 250–271. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijer.2006.06.005
  • *Li, Y., Xu, L., Liu, L., Lv, Y., Wang, Y., & Huntsinger, C. S. (2016). Can preschool socioeconomic composition moderate relationships between family environment and Chinese children's early academic and social outcomes? Children and Youth Services Review, 60, 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2015.11.009
  • Liberati, A., Atman, D., Tetzlaff, J., Mulrow, C., Gøtzsche, P., Ioannidis, J., Clarke, M., Devereaux, P., Kleijnen, J., & Moher, D. (2009). The PRISMA statement for reporting systematic reviews and meta-analyses of studies that evaluate health care interventions: Explanation and elaboration. PLOS Medicine, 6, 1–28. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1000100
  • *Lockl, K., Ebert, S., & Weinert, S. (2017). Predicting school achievement from early theory of mind: Differential effects on achievement tests and teacher ratings. Learning and Individual Differences, 53, 93–102. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2016.11.007
  • *Lombardi, C. M., Casey, B. M., Thomson, D., & Nguyen, H. N. (2017). Maternal support of young children’s planning and spatial concept learning as predictors of later math (and reading) achievement. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 41, 114–125. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2017.07.004
  • Lonnemann, J., & Hasselhorn, M. (2018). Early mathematics education: Current research trends and perspectives. Frühe Bildung, 7(3), 129–134. https://doi.org/10.1026/2191-9186/a000379
  • Mähler, C., Petermann, U., & Greve, W. (2017). Social-emotional and cognitive skills as regulation competencies. Kindheit und Entwicklung, 26(1), 1–6. https://doi.org/10.1026/0942-5403/a000210
  • *Martin, R. B., Cirino, P. T., Sharp, C., & Barnes, M. (2014). Number and counting skills in kindergarten as predictors of grade 1 mathematical skills. Learning and Individual Differences, 34, 12–23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2014.05.006
  • Marx, E. (2006). Does a child’s language system benefit from other domains of cognitive development? A study of the relationship between language acquisition and inductive reasoning. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 38(3), 139–145. https://doi.org/10.1026/0049-8637.38.3.139
  • Marx, E., & Keller, K. (2010). Effects of an Inductive Reasoning Training on the Development of Language and Intelligence in Preschoolers and First Graders from Socially Disadvantaged Districts. Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie, 24(2), 139–146. https://doi.org/10.1024/1010-0652/a000011
  • *McClelland, M. M., Cameron, C. E., Connor, C. M., Farris, C. L., Jewkes, A. M., & Morrisson, F. J. (2007). Links between behavioral regulation and preschoolers’ literacy, vocabulary, and math skills. Developmental Psychology, 43(4), 947–959. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.43.4.947
  • *McClelland, M. M., Cameron, C. E., Duncan, R., Bowles, P. P., Acock, A. C., Miao, A., & Pratt, M. E. (2014). Predictors of early growth in academic achievement: The head-toes-knees-shoulders task. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1–14. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00599
  • *McCormick, M. P., O'Connor, E. E., & Barnes, S. P. (2016). Mother–child attachment styles and math and reading skills in middle childhood: The mediating role of children’s exploration and engagement. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 36, 295–306. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2016.01.011
  • McCormick, M. P., Neuhaus, R., Horn, E. P., O’Connor, E. E., White, H. I., Harding, S., Cappella, E., & McClowry, S. (2019). Long-term effects of social-emotional learning on receipt of special education and grade retention: Evidence from a randomized trial of INSIGHTS. AERA Open, 5(3), 1–21. https://doi.org/10.1177/2332858419867290
  • *McLear, C., Trentacosta, C. J., & Smith-Darden, J. (2016). Child self-regulation, parental secure base scripts, and at-risk kindergartners’ academic achievement. Early Education and Development, 27(4), 440–456. https://doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2016.1091972
  • *Merz, E., Zucker, T. A., Landry, S. H., Williams, J. M., Assel, M., Taylor, H., Lonigan, C., Phillips, B. M., Clancy-Menchetti, J., Barnes, M. A., Eisenberg, N., & de Villiers, J. (2015). Parenting predictors of cognitive skills and emotion knowledge in socioeconomically disadvantaged preschoolers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 132, 14–31. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2014.11.010
  • *Miller-Cotto, D., & Byrnes, J. P. (2020). What’s the best way to characterize the relationship between working memory and achievement? An initial examination of competing theories. Journal of Educational Psychology, 112(5), 1074–1084. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000395
  • *Moll, K., Snowling, M. J., Göbel, S. M., & Hulme, C. (2015). Early language and executive skills predict variations in number and arithmetic skills in children at family-risk of dyslexia and typically developing controls. Learning and Instruction, 38, 53–62. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2015.03.004
  • *Morgan, P. L., Farkas, G., Hillemeier, M., Pun, W. H., & Maczuga, S. (2019). Kindergarten children's executive functions predict their second‐grade academic achievement and behavior. Child Development, 90(5), 1802–1816. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13095
  • *Mou, Y., Bertletti, I., & Hyde, D. C. (2018). What counts in preschool number knowledge? A Bayes factor analytic approach toward theoretical model development. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 166, 116–133. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.07.016
  • *Nanu, C., McMullen, J., Munck, P., & Hannula-Sormunen, M. H. (2018). Spontaneous focusing on numerosity in preschool as a predictor of mathematical skills and knowledge in the fifth grade. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 169, 42–58. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.12.011
  • *Neuenschwander, R., Röthlisberger, M., Cimeli, P., & Roebers, C. (2012). How do different aspects of self-regulation predict successful adaptation to school? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 113(3), 353–371. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.07.004
  • *Nevo, E., & Breznitz, Z. (2011). Assessment of working memory components at 6 years of age as predictors of reading achievements a year later. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 109(1), 73–90. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2010.09.010
  • *Nguyen, T., & Duncan, G. J. (2019). Kindergarten components of executive function and third grade achievement: A national study. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 46, 49–61. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.05.006
  • *Nguyen, T., Duncan, R. J., & Bailey, D. (2019). Theoretical and methodological implications of associations between executive function and mathematics in early childhood. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 58, 376–287. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2019.04.002
  • *Niklas, F., & Schneider, W. (2014). Casting the die before the die is cast: the importance of the home numeracy environment for preschool children. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 29, 327–345. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10212-013-0201-6
  • *Niklas, F., & Schneider, W. (2017). Home learning environment and development of child competencies from kindergarten until the end of elementary school. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 49, 263–274. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2017.03.006
  • *Niklas, F., & Tayler, C. (2018). Room quality and composition matters: Children's verbal and numeracy abilities in Australian early childhood settings. Learning and Instruction, 54, 114–124. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2017.08.006
  • *Noël, M.-P. (2009). Counting on working memory when learning to count and to add: A preschool study. Developmental Psychology, 45(6), 1630–1643. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016224
  • OECD. (2021). Starting Strong IV. Supporting Meaningful Interactions in Early Childhood Education and Care (Vol. Starting Strong). Paris: OECD Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1787/f47a06ae-en
  • Offer-Boljahn, H., Hövel, D., & Hennemann, T. (2019). Multicomponent interventions in early child education: A systematic review. Psychoeducational Assessment, Intervention and Rehabilitation, 1(1), 31–44. https://doi.org/10.30436/PAIR19-01
  • *Passolunghi, M. C., Lanfranchi, S., Altoè, G., & Sollazzo, N. (2015). Early numerical abilities and cognitive skills in kindergarten children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 135, 25–42. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.02.001
  • *Passolunghi, M. C., Vercelloni, B., & Schadee, H. (2007). The precursors of mathematics learning: Working memory, phonological ability and numerical competence. Cognitive Development, 22(2), 165–184. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2006.09.001
  • *Paul, J. M., Gray, S. A., Butterworth, B. L., & Reeve, R. A. (2019). Reading and math tests differentially predict number transcoding and number fact speed longitudinally: A random intercept cross-lagged panel approach. Journal of Educational Psychology, 111(2), 299–313. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000287
  • Peijnenborgh, J., Hurks, P., Aldenkamp, A., Vles, J., & Hendriksen, J. (2015). Efficacy of working memory training in children and adolescents with learning disabilities: A review study and meta-analysis. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 26(5-6), 645–672. https://doi.org/10.1080/09602011.2015.1026356
  • *Perry, K. E., Donohue, K. M., & Weinstein, R. S. (2007). Teaching practices and the promotion of achievement and adjustment in first grade. Journal of School Psychology, 45(3), 269–292. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2007.02.005
  • *Preßler, A.-L., Könen, T., Hasselhorn, M., & Krajewski, K. (2014). Cognitive preconditions of early reading and spelling: a latent-variable approach with longitudinal data. Reading and Writing, 27, 383–406. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-013-9449-0
  • *Puccioni, J., Baker, E. R., & Froiland, J. M. (2019). Academic socialization and the transition to kindergarten: Parental beliefs about school readiness and involvement. Infant and Child Development, 28(6), 1–25. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.2154
  • Puolakanaho, A., Ahonen, T., Aro, M., Eklund, K., Leppänen, P., Poikkeus, A.-M., Tolvanen, A., Torppa, M., & Lyytinen, H. (2008). Developmental links of very early phonological and language skills to second grade reading outcomes. Strong to accuracy but only minor to fluency. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 41(4), 353–370. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219407311747
  • *Purpura, D. J., & Logan, J. A. (2015). The nonlinear relations of the approximate number system and mathematical language to early mathematics development. Developmental Psychology, 51(12), 1717–1724. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000055
  • *Purpura, D. J., & Simms, V. (2018). Approximate Number System development in preschool: What factors predict change? Cognitive Development, 45, 31–39. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.11.001
  • *Purpura, D. J., Logan, J. A., Hassinger-Das, B., & Napoli, A. R. (2017). Why do early mathematics skills predict later reading? The role of mathematical language. Developmental Psychology, 53(9), 1633–1642. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000375
  • *Rasmussen, C., & Bisanz, J. (2005). Representation and working memory in early arithmetic. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 91(2), 137–157. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2005.01.004
  • Reinelt, T., Samdan, G., Kiel, N., & Petermann, F. (2019). Predicting Externalizing Behavior Problems in Early Childhood: Evidence From Longitudinal Studies. Kindheit und Entwicklung, 28(1), 19–32. https://doi.org/10.1026/0942-5403/a000268
  • Rossbach, H.-G., Sechtig, J., & Freund, U. (2010). Empirische Evaluation des Modellversuchs "Kindergarten der Zukunft - KiDZ". Ergebnisse der Kindergartenphase [Empirical Evaluation of the Pilot Project „Kindergarten of the Future – KiDZ“. Results of the Kindergarten Years]. University of Bamberg Press.
  • *Sáez, L., Sidler Folsom, J., Al Otaiba, S., & Schatschneider, C. (2012). Relations among student attention behaviors, teacher practices, and beginning word reading skill. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 45(5), 418–432. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219411431243
  • *Sasser, T. R., Bierman, K. L., & Heinrichs, B. (2015). Executive functioning and school adjustment: The mediational role of pre-kindergarten learning-related behaviors. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 30, 70–79. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2014.09.001
  • *Schmerold, K., Bock, A., Peterson, M., Leaf, B., Vennergrund, K., & Pasnak, R. (2017). The relations between patterning, executive function, and mathematics. The Journal of Psychology, 151(2), 207–228. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223980.2016.1252708
  • *Schmitt, S. A., Geldhof, J., Purpura, D., Duncan, R., & McClelland, M. M. (2017). Examining the relations between executive function, math, and literacy during the transition to kindergarten: A multi-analytic approach. Journal of Educational Psychology, 109(8), 1120–1140. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000193
  • Schuchardt, K., & Kuhn, J.-T. (2019). Komorbidität von Lernschwierigkeiten. Lernen und Lernstörungen, 8(1), 5–6. https://doi.org/10.1024/2235-0977/a000251
  • Schulze, S., & Kuhl, J. (2019). Mathematical learning support and integrated working memory training. Lernen und Lernstörungen, 8(1), 47–59. https://doi.org/10.1024/2235-0977/a000229
  • *Schwarz, M., & Shaul, S. (2018). Towards a better understanding of the link between executive functions, early literacy, and emergent mathematical abilities. Written Language & Literacy, 21(2), 238–268. https://doi.org/10.1075/wll.00016.sch
  • Sedlmeier, P., & Renkewitz, F. (2013). Forschungsmethoden und Statistik für Psychologen und Sozialwissenschaftler [Research Methods and Statistics for Psychologists and Social Scientists] (2nd ed.). Pearson.
  • *Segers, E., Kleemans, T., & Verhoeven, L. (2015). Role of parent literacy and numeracy expectations and activities in predicting early numeracy skills. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 17(2-3), 219–236. https://doi.org/10.1080/10986065.2015.1016819
  • *Sims, D. M., Purpura, D. J., & Lonigan, C. J. (2016). The relation between inattentive and hyperactive/impulsive behaviors and early mathematics skills. Journal of Attention Disorders, 20(8), 704–714. https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054712464390
  • *Skwarchuk, S.-L., Sowinski, C., & LeFevre, J.-A. (2014). Formal and informal home learning activities in relation to children’s early numeracy and literacy skills: The development of a home numeracy model. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 121, 63–84. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2013.11.006
  • *Smith-Adcock, S., Leite, W., & Amatea, E. (2019). A model of parenting risk and resilience, social-emotional readiness, and reading achievement in kindergarten children from low-income families model. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 28, 2826–2841. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-019-01462-0
  • *Son, S.-H. C., Choi, J. Y., & Kwon, K.-A. (2019). Reciprocal associations between inhibitory control and early academic skills: Evidence from a nationally representative sample of Head Start children. Early Education and Development, 30(4), 456–477. https://doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2019.1572382
  • *Soto-Calvo, E., Simmons, F. R., Willis, C., & Adams, A.-M. (2015). Identifying the cognitive predictors of early counting and calculation skills: Evidence from a longitudinal study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 140, 16–37. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.06.011
  • *Stipek, D., & Valentino, R. A. (2015). Early childhood memory and attention as predictors of academic growth trajectories. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107(3), 771–788. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000004
  • *Tindal, G., Irvin, S., & Nese, J. F. (2015). Skills for children entering kindergarten. Educational Assessment, 20(4), 297–319. https://doi.org/10.1080/10627197.2015.1093929
  • *Trentacosta, C. J., & Izard, C. E. (2007). Kindergarten children’s emotion competence as a predictor of their academic competence in first grade. Emotion, 7(1), 77–88. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.7.1.77
  • *Vandenbroucke, L., Verschueren, K., Desoete, A., Aunio, P., Ghesquière, P., & Baeyens, D. (2018). Crossing the bridge to elementary school: The development of children’s working memory components in relation to teacher-student relationships and academic achievement. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 42, 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2017.08.004
  • Viljaranta, J., Lerkkanen, L.-K., Poikkeus, A.-M., Aunola, K., & Nurmi, J.-E. (2009). Cross-lagged relations between task motivation and performance in arithmetic and literacy in kindergarten. Learning and Instruction, 19(4), 335–344. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2008.06.011
  • *Viterbori, P., Usai, C., Traverso, L., & De Franchis, V. (2015). How preschool executive functioning predicts several aspects of math achievement in Grades 1 and 3: A longitudinal study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 140, 38–55. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.06.014
  • *Weiland, C., Ulvestad, K., Sachs, J., & Yoshikawa, H. (2013). Associations between classroom quality and children’s vocabulary and executive function skills in an urban public prekindergarten program. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 28(2), 199–209. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2012.12.002
  • *Welsh, J., Nix, R., Blair, C., Bierman, K., & Nelson, K. (2010). The development of cognitive skills and gains in academic school readiness for children from low-income families. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102(1), 43–53. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016738
  • *Willoughby, M., Blair, C., Wirth, R. J., & Greenberg, M. (2012). The measurement of executive function at age 5: Psychometric properties and relationship to academic achievement. Psychological Assessment, 24, 226–236. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025361
  • Wustmann Seiler, C., Müller, E., & Simoni, H. (2017). The protective role of childcare quality for behavioral adjustment in 3- to 5-year-old children. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 49, 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1026/0049-8637/a000162
  • *Zhang, X., & Lin, D. (2018). Cognitive precursors of word reading versus arithmetic competencies in young Chinese children. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 42, 55–65. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2017.08.006

License

This is an open access article distributed under the Creative Commons Attribution License which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.